News:

NOTICE: Posting schedule is irregular. I hope to get back to a regular schedule as the day-job allows.


Friday, September 9, 2011

http://teddysratlab.blogspot.com [Full link to blog for email clients.][FT:C44]


I haven’t stopped to answer questions based on the Lab Rats’ Guide to the Brain for quite some time.  I have received comments in the blog, as well as in the Baen’s Bar online community.  Unfortunately I haven’t been entirely timely in answering the ones that require more than just a quick answer – but I do save them for later use.
So for the next several days I will work through the backlog and answer your questions about the brain.  So without further ado…
[squeak]
What’s that, Ratley?
[squeak]
Um, yeah, I suppose I should.
[squeak]
Right.  Ratley is going to gather the other LabRats around for this.   Since I still have a scientific conference and Dragon*Con this week, I’m going to let them tackle the mail while I finish my two presentations for the meeting.  So, I’ll just set up this automatic translator and get back to fixing slides.
[squeak... er, excuse me.  Hi.  I'm Ratley, and I'm supervisor of the LabRats.  What Speaker seems to have forgotten is that it has traditionally been the job of the LabRats to answer the mail.  The fact that he hasn't let us out of the lab much these past few months is one of the reasons we've gotten so far behind in reading the mail.  We each have our different areas of expertise, so the LabRats will share the task of answering questions.  For the first couple of questions, I'm going to let Ratso answer.  He's pretty smart, even if he does prefer to spend too much time in the kitchen and talk like ... well, a rat!]
Ratso:  Yo, Ratboy, I *resemble* dat remark!  Okay, on wit' da program heah:  Chris writes:  “I’m working on a story where my character has to get used to his new super abilities.  Just how fast does the brain process?  If thoughts, movements and reflexes were to be sped up, how hard would it be for my character to cope and learn how to use them?"
Well, Chris-boy, it's like dis – da brain is only as fast as its parts.  To understan' how fast dat can be, ya gotta figger out how fast is each link in da chain.  Da neurons, well, dey's pretty fast.  Takin' the time from the first chemical activation of a synapse to when da action potential starts is about 2 milliseconds.  Action potentials (http://teddysratlab.blogspot.com/2011/02/piece-of-action-potential.html) travel to the other end of da neuron at about 10 meters per sec.  If da neuron is a *long* sensory or motor control neuron for da body, it has a special insulatin' coating called "Myelin" dat speeds up da action potential to about 100 meters/sec. Once da action reaches da end of the neuron, neurotransmitter chemicals are release to start da process all over again in da next neuron.  Da chemical part takes about 2-3 millseconds. 
So each link in da chain, dat is, each neuron "synapse" takes about 5 millseconds, and it takes an action potential less than 10 milliseconds to travel either from brain to brain area, or from brain to hands – about 20 milliseconds to get to da feet.  Dat's pretty slick, if ya ask me!  Multiply dat by about 4 or 5 times for connections between neurons and parts of brain controlling an action.  I like connections.  I'm a connections kind of guy myself!
So yeah, da brain can process pretty fast, almost anything ya wanna do can be completed in about 100 milliseconds.  If'n ya wants to write a story wit' everything speeded up, ya can prob'ly get about 10 times da speed outta just makin' the chemical and electrical connections more efficient, but like any good exercise, ya gotta learn ta use it.  In fact, it'll be jus' like any new skill: practice, practice, Practice!
***
Ratley: Thank you, Ratso.  Perhaps you should get back to the stockroom, there's a fellow named Guido back there saying something about kitchen supplied that fell off of a truck?
For the next question, perhaps we should defer to our resident master of motor skill, Nestor:
Nestor:  Why thank you old chap, that's mighty kind of you.  Be a good lad and hand me that letter then?  Ah yes.  In response to “The Six Senses (and maybe more…)” Anonymous asks:  “What about the ability to do complex tasks requiring fine motor skill, with your hands out of sight and in all sorts of odd positions? I do that quite often when working on cars and other mechanical things. It's like I can "see" with my hands, often it helps if I close my eyes.”
Dear Sir:
What you are describing is commonly misnamed as "muscle memory."  A skill practiced to a point where it becomes automatic.  The muscles move the way they are supposed to every time, you don't have to think about it. 
In reality, this should be termed "cerebellar memory."  The cerebellum is the part of the brain most responsible for coordinating movements.  It is quite heavily connected to all those parts of the brain involved in a movement or reflex.  For instance – when moving your arm, you must move the arm muscles and joints, but you also turn your head, shift your weight on your feet, twist slightly at the waist and often look at the movement.  If you don't look, you'll probably do it ok, but you may not be quite as accurate in the motion.  Now try to move your arm while keeping the rest of your body completely still!  Quite a bit of bother, no?  That is because the brain issues all of those commands, coordinated by the cerebellum, and when you consciously override the reflexes, the cerebellum requires all new programming.  Sort of like learning to raise a single eyebrow.
Soldiers and athletes understand the concept very well.  Actions must be learned so well that they are automatic – however it really is the brain learning, and not the muscles.  As for your question, it's all the same, righty-o?  Because we have learned how our muscles move while we watch them, it is right simple to imagine those movements, then perform them with our eyes closed.  You "see" with your hands because the brain uses all of the information about movement and position and then calls upon memory to fill in the missing pieces.  All of the connections from the cerebellum to the motor and sensory areas of the brain, plus the connections to visual and auditory tracking centers in the brainstem provide a "map" of the body in 3-D space.  Quite amazing, if I may say so myself. 
Now, if you will excuse me, my tail seems to be caught in a… Oh bother!
***
Ratley:  Thanks, Nestor.  We'll send the Jaws of Life in for you, don't worry.
Sandra writes:  Dear Speaker:
My daughter's domesticated pet rats have been vocalizing a lot lately. The other day I walked into her room and told her it was time to get up. A rat (in the cage) poked its head out of its sleeping hammock and said *squeak-squeak*. Later, as the two rats were out of their cage, playing, they froze when the big black dog stepped into the room. I sternly told the dog to leave (she's not allowed in that room) and after the dog left, the biggest rat rushed to my side, staring at the door, and growled and squeaked. I've never heard her (the rat) make these sounds before, and it sounded like she was scolding the dog.
I realize we humans tend to anthropomorphize our pets, but could these rats be trying to communicate? And what about the personalities these rats seem to have? For many people, rats are vermin and pests; for cats and dogs they are prey. How can these rats be so charming?
[squeak!]
Ratley:  What's that, Ratface?  Cousins you say?  Yes, Indeed.  They must be!
Dear Aunt Sandra – (for if the rats are our cousins, you must be our Aunt!) – domestication of any animal is a learned process.  What you are describing represents two aspects of learning that apply to many animals.  If you have ever watched circus animals, movie dogs or the "Walrus, Otter, Sea Lion" show at Sea World you have seen the results of what is called "operant conditioning."  Most folks should recall reading or learning about Pavlov's experiments in classical conditioning.  Pavlov rang a bell simultaneous with the smell/taste of food.  The dog salivated at the food stimulus, and eventually learned to salivate at the sound of the bell alone.  In behavioral terms, we talk about an "unconditioned stimulus" (UCS) – that is, a stimulus that produces the desired response without learning.  In the Pavlov example, smell of food is the UCS and salivation is the response.  Then there is the "Conditioned Stimulus" (CS) – this is a stimulus (bell) that we *want* to produce the same response as the UCS.  So we present UCS and CS together for a *lot* of repetitions – usually with the CS occurring before the UCS.  Eventually the conditioned response (salivation) starts to occur after the bell, but before the smell of food.  Once that happens, we call it a "conditioned response" (CR). Once our experiment begins to look like this:  CS – CR – then UCS, we have conditioned the animal and can even omit the smell of food (UCS) and still get salivation (CR) just by ringing the bell (CS).  Voila - Pavlov's classic experiment in behavioral conditioning, also known as "classical conditioning."   
Operant conditioning differs in that we actually teach the animals a chain of UCS stimuli, and those stimuli require the animal to *do* something.  In the Sea World show example, the sea lion is conditioned to know that ringing a bell means a food reward will be given.  This is classical conditioning, but then we add a twist – now the sea lion gets a chance to explore the bell – if it rings the bell itself, it gets a fish!  This is the first essential step in operant training, the animal makes its own response and receives a reward.  To make more complex behaviors, we just train in one preceding step at a time – before the sea lion can ring the bell, it must climb some steps.  So in this step we have two operant phases: (1) climb steps, in order to be able to (2) ring bell, to receive the reward.  It's just like training dogs, horses, or even people.  Once enough steps are "chained" together, our sea lion becomes the star act and appears to be behaving just like a human – but it's only conditioning at work.
Your pet rats have become conditioned.  They know which humans feed them, take them out for exercise, and protect them from predators.  This is where the second aspect of learning comes in: dominance.  Normally, dogs are predators to rats, as are humans.  Trust me.  We lab rats know all about that!  However, in caring for our cousins, and keeping the other predators away (!) we come to recognize you humans as the dominant creatures in our environment.  As long as we remain conditioned to you, and you protect us, we'll act submissive to you.
Now submission comes in many forms.  Speaker tells us stories of working with primates.  If there's one big mean monkey in a colony, the other monkeys are submissive, and most will not challenge the "Alpha."  However, if the Alpha attacks a smaller monkey and a human intervenes, the lesser monkeys look to the human as Alpha (and sometimes even the Alpha will defer to the human).  The human is the more dominant creature and protector.  In animal behavior terms, that's usually called "mate," but is also "companion" with the commonly domesticated animals such as dogs and horses. 
So what you are seeing is a bit of anthropomorphization (hmm, Ratfink must have left that word lying around here) but also a result of the conditioning and dominance hierarchy among animals.
But then again, it might just be that our cousins are a bit smarter than you think!
***
Oops, here comes Speaker, time to get back to work.
Speaker:  Thanks, Ratley.  My meetings are over, as is Dragon*Con.  There's a few more letters in the mailbag here, and I'll try to get to those next week.  I hope the answers were informative, but you can never tell with LabRats!
Until next time….

No comments:

Post a Comment

Please add comment - no links, spammers will be banned.